VARIATIONS IN COMPLEXITY A rough indication of physiological cell types found in the different layers of the striate cortex. As we would expect, cells near the input end of the cortex, in layer 4, show less complicated behavior than cells near the output. In the monkey, as noted in this chapter, cells in layer 4Cß, which receive input from the upper four (parvocellular) geniculate layers, all seem to have center-surround properties, without orientation selectivity. In layer 4Ca, whose input is from the ventral (magnocellular) pair of geniculate layers, some cells have center-surround fields, but others seem to be orientation-specific, with simple receptive fields. Farther downstream, in the layers above and below 4C, the great majority of cells are complex. End-stopping occurs in about 20 percent of cells in layers 2 and 3 but seldom occurs elsewhere. On the whole, then, we find a loose correlation between complexity and distance along the visual path, measured in numbers of synapses. Stating that most cells above and below layer 4 are complex glosses over major layer-to- layer differences, because complex cells are far from all alike. They all have in common the defining characteristic of complex cells--they respond throughout their receptive field to a properly oriented moving line regardless of its exact position--but they differ in other ways. We can distinguish four subtypes that tend to be housed in different layers. In layers 2 and 3, most complex cells respond progressively better the longer the slit (they show length summation), and the response becomes weaker when the line exceeds a critical length only if a cell is end stopped. For cells in layer 5, short slits, covering only a small part of the length of a receptive field, work about as well as long ones; the receptive fields are much larger than the fields of cells in layers 2 and 3. For cells in layer 6, in contrast, the longer an optimally oriented line is, the stronger are the responses, until the line spans the entire length of the field, which is several times greater than the width (the distance over which a moving line evokes responses). The field is thus long and narrow. We can conclude that axons running from layers 5, 6, and 2 and 3 to different targets in the brain (the superior culliculus, geniculate, the other visual cortical areas) must carry somewhat different kinds of visual information. In summary, from layer to layer we find differences in the way cells behave that seem more fundamental than differences, say, in optimal orientation or in ocular dominance. The most obvious of these layer-to-layer differences is in response complexity, which reflects the simple anatomical fact that some layers are closer than others to the input.